본문 바로가기
AI

Working Memory

by STARPOPO 2025. 1. 8.
반응형
Unlike traditional short-term memory, which is often viewed as a passive storage system, working memory is characterized by its active processing capabilities, allowing individuals to manage multiple streams of information simultaneously. This distinction underscores its importance across numerous domains, including education, where research indicates a strong correlation between working memory capacity and academic performance in areas such as literacy and mathematics.

 
 
 
 

Summary

 
 
Working memory (WM) is a cognitive system responsible for temporarily holding and manipulating information essential for various cognitive tasks, including problem-solving, reasoning, and language comprehension. Unlike traditional short-term memory, which is often viewed as a passive storage system, working memory is characterized by its active processing capabilities, allowing individuals to manage multiple streams of information simultaneously.[1][2] This distinction underscores its importance across numerous domains, including education, where research indicates a strong correlation between working memory capacity and academic performance in areas such as literacy and mathematics.[3][4]
 
 
The conceptualization of working memory has evolved significantly since its introduction in the 1970s, notably through the Multicomponent Working Memory Model proposed by Alan Baddeley and Graham Hitch. This model identifies several components, including the central executive, phonological loop, visuospatial sketchpad, and episodic buffer, which together facilitate the complex interactions required for cognitive functioning.[1][5] In contrast, alternative models, such as Cowan's embedded-processes model and recent oscillatory models, emphasize different aspects of how working memory operates, indicating a rich and ongoing debate within the field of cognitive psychology regarding its underlying mechanisms.[6][7]
 
 
Despite advancements in understanding, working memory remains a topic of contention, particularly concerning its distinctions from short-term memory and the neurobiological substrates that support its functions. Some researchers challenge the traditional view of persistent neuronal activity in working memory, proposing instead that information maintenance may occur through discrete neural oscillations.[8][9] Moreover, the practical implications of working memory are significant, as deficits in this cognitive domain are observed in populations with conditions such as ADHD and dyslexia, highlighting the necessity for targeted educational strategies and interventions.[10][11]
 
 
In summary, working memory is a pivotal cognitive construct with profound implications for learning and performance across various contexts. As research continues to unfold, understanding its components, influences, and practical applications remains essential for advancing both theoretical and applied cognitive science.[12][13]
 
 

History

 
 
The concept of working memory has evolved significantly since its inception, with various theories and models contributing to its understanding. The foundational work in this area can be traced back to the 1970s when Baddeley and Hitch introduced the Multicomponent Working Memory Model, which proposed that working memory is not a unitary system but consists of multiple components, including the central executive, phonological loop, and visuospatial sketchpad[1]. This model marked a departure from the traditional view of short-term memory as a singular construct, emphasizing the complexity and multifaceted nature of memory processes[2].
 
 
Subsequent research has built upon Baddeley and Hitch's model, leading to the inclusion of additional components, such as the episodic buffer, which was proposed to integrate information across different modalities[2]. The dynamic interplay between these components has been supported by empirical evidence, suggesting that working memory plays a crucial role in various cognitive tasks, including problem-solving and language comprehension[2].
 
 
However, the prevailing theories have not gone unchallenged. Recent studies have questioned the traditional models, particularly the notion of persistent neuronal activity as a basis for working memory. Alternative proposals, such as the spiking working memory model suggested by Fiebig and Lansner, argue that working memory processes may manifest in discrete oscillatory bursts rather than continuous activation[3]. This shift in perspective highlights the ongoing debates and developments within the field of cognitive psychology regarding the mechanisms underlying working memory[4].
 
 
Despite the complexities and disagreements among researchers, the importance of working memory in educational contexts and its implications for learning and cognitive performance have garnered significant attention. Studies have shown that working memory capacity is closely linked to various academic outcomes, emphasizing the necessity of understanding its role in both theoretical and practical applications[5][6]. As research continues to unfold, the understanding of working memory is likely to evolve, reflecting advancements in cognitive neuroscience and psychology.
 
 

Components of Working Memory

 
Working memory (WM) is comprised of several components that collaboratively function to store and manipulate information temporarily. The most widely accepted model of working memory, proposed by Baddeley and Hitch, includes the Central Executive, Phonological Loop, visuospatial Sketchpad, and the Episodic Buffer.
 
 

Phonological Loop

 
The Phonological Loop is responsible for processing verbal and auditory information. It is divided into two subcomponents: the Phonological Store (or "Inner Ear"), which retains speech sounds in their temporal order, and the Articulatory Process (or "Inner Voice"), which allows for subvocal rehearsal of this information to prevent decay[7][8]. The Phonological Loop plays a significant role in tasks involving language and is crucial for vocabulary acquisition, especially in early childhood[7].
 
 

Central Executive

 
The Central Executive serves as the primary control system in the working memory model. It regulates attention and coordinates the activities of the other components, allowing for the integration of information from various sources. Its functions include updating and coding incoming information, binding information into coherent episodes, and managing the interaction between working memory and long-term memory[8]. The Central Executive is modality-free, enabling it to process information in any form, although it has a limited capacity and can handle only a small amount of information at once[9].
 
 

Episodic Buffer

 
 
Introduced later as an additional component, the Episodic Buffer acts as a temporary storage system that integrates information from the Phonological Loop and Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad along with relevant long-term memory[9]. This integration enables the formation of multi-dimensional representations, allowing for a more cohesive recall of experiences[9]. The Episodic Buffer has a limited capacity, estimated to manage around four chunks of information simultaneously, and employs multi-modal coding to process various forms of data[9].
 
 
These components work together to facilitate complex cognitive tasks, supporting functions such as learning, problem-solving, and decision-making, by allowing individuals to retain and manipulate information in real time[10].
 
 

Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad

 
 
The Visuo-Spatial Sketchpad is dedicated to the manipulation of visual and spatial information. It functions as a separate storage system that does not interfere with the operations of the Phonological Loop[8]. This component is further divided into two sub-systems: the Visual Cache, which stores information about form and color, and the Inner Scribe, which arranges objects in the visual field[8]. This sketchpad allows individuals to visualize scenes, navigate spaces, and recall visual memories, such as artwork or landscapes[8].
 
 

Theories and Models

 
 
Working memory (WM) has been conceptualized through various models that attempt to explain its structure and function. Among the most influential is the model proposed by Baddeley and Hitch in 1974, which introduced a multi-component framework consisting of a central executive and two domain-specific storage systems: the phonological loop and the visuospatial sketchpad. This model was later expanded to include a fourth component, the episodic buffer, which integrates information across domains and links it to long-term memory[1][11][7].
 
 

Baddeley and Hitch's Model

 
 
The Baddeley and Hitch model was formulated in response to the limitations of the Modal Model of Memory proposed by Atkinson and Shiffrin, which viewed short-term memory as a passive storage space. In contrast, Baddeley and Hitch emphasized the active processing and manipulation of information, leading to the adoption of the term "working memory" to reflect these functions[12][13]. The model posits that the central executive serves as a supervisory system, coordinating the flow of information between the slave systems. The phonological loop is responsible for verbal and auditory information, while the visuospatial sketchpad handles visual and spatial data[11][7].
 
 

Cowan's Embedded-Processes Model

 
 
In contrast to Baddeley and Hitch, Cowan (1999, 2010) proposed the embedded-processes model of working memory, which focuses on the cognitive processes involved in task performance, such as attention and information retrieval from long-term memory. Cowan's model identifies four key elements: the central executive, long-term memory, activated memory, and the focus of attention. This model suggests that working memory is an activated subset of long-term memory, emphasizing the fluid nature of memory retrieval and the role of attention in maintaining and manipulating information[1][14].
 
 

Oscillatory Models

 
 
Another approach to understanding working memory involves oscillatory models, which posit that neural oscillations play a crucial role in maintaining information in working memory. These models suggest that the synchronization of brain activity across different regions supports the temporary storage and manipulation of information, highlighting the dynamic interplay between cognitive processes and neural mechanisms[15].
 
 

Limitations and Future Directions

 
 
Despite the advancements in our understanding of working memory, several limitations and ambiguities persist in the literature. One ongoing debate is the distinction between working memory and short-term memory (STM), with some researchers advocating for their separation as distinct constructs, while others argue they are part of a continuum[14]. This lack of consensus is notable given the extensive research conducted on both concepts. Future research may benefit from clarifying these definitions and exploring the relationships between different models to achieve a more integrated understanding of working memory[14][11].
 
 

Measurement and Assessment

 
 
Working memory (WM) capacity can be evaluated through various tasks, with a commonly used method being the dual-task paradigm. This approach combines a memory span measure with a concurrent processing task, sometimes referred to as a "complex span". The original version of this task, known as "reading span," was developed by Daneman and Carpenter in 1980, where subjects read a series of sentences and were tasked with remembering the final word of each sentence, ultimately recalling those words in their correct order[13]. Alternative tasks, such as "n-back" tasks, are also employed to assess individual differences in working memory ability. In an n-back task, participants must identify when the current item in a sequence matches a previous item, which can be designed in varying levels of complexity, such as 2-back tasks[10].
 
 
To gain a comprehensive understanding of working memory, a multi-method, multi-informant approach is often recommended, as WM is recognized as a multi-component and dynamic construct[1]. Standardized achievement tests are frequently utilized to measure students’ academic skills and knowledge systematically, emphasizing the need for comparable assessment measures across different educational contexts[1]. Despite this, there is no universal assessment for academic performance, highlighting the variability based on age, context, and geography[1].
 
 
The interplay between working memory and academic performance has been a focal point of research. While some studies suggest that WM training may not directly translate to improved academic outcomes, others indicate significant correlations between working memory capacity and cognitive abilities, such as fluid reasoning and reading skills[3][5]. Specifically, neuroimaging studies have shown that intelligence tests activate similar neural networks associated with working memory, reinforcing the connection between these cognitive domains[3].
 
 

Factors Affecting Working Memory

 
 
Working memory is influenced by various factors, including cognitive load, age, and the nature of tasks, which all play significant roles in its performance and capacity.
 
 

Cognitive Load

 
 
Cognitive load refers to the amount of mental effort being used in the working memory. It depends on two variables: the rate at which processing tasks require individual steps to be completed, and the duration of each step. For example, a task that requires adding digits rapidly imposes a higher cognitive load than a task with more extended intervals between additions. Research by Barrouillet and colleagues has shown that memory for lists of items is not solely dependent on the number of processing steps or the total time spent processing, but rather on the cognitive load imposed during the task[13].
 
 

Age

 
The capacity of working memory develops gradually throughout childhood and typically begins to decline in old age. Children experience significant growth in working memory between the ages of 4 and 15, which correlates with developmental changes in brain structures such as the prefrontal cortex[1][3]. In contrast, older adults often show reduced working memory performance, influenced by age-related cognitive decline and neural efficiency issues, such as the Compensation-Related Utilization of Neural Circuits Hypothesis (CRUNCH), which posits that older individuals may over-activate certain neural circuits to compensate for cognitive deficits[13][3].
 
 

Nature of Tasks

 
The type of task being performed also significantly affects working memory. Research indicates that verbal and visuospatial tasks engage different sensory mechanisms, influencing performance outcomes. For instance, individuals may perform differently on auditory versus visual n-back tasks due to the inherent nature of the information being processed[8][3]. This variability highlights the importance of understanding the specific demands of working memory tasks and how they relate to individual performance across different contexts.
 
 

Applications

 
Working memory (WM) is crucial in various educational contexts, significantly impacting students' learning and performance in subjects such as mathematics and literacy. As the brain’s “workbench,” WM enables individuals to temporarily hold and manipulate information, thereby facilitating complex problem-solving and comprehension tasks[16]. In mathematics, for example, working memory helps students keep track of numbers and operations, which is essential for tackling complex calculations[16]. Similarly, in literacy, it plays a vital role in reading comprehension by allowing learners to retain ideas while processing sentences and paragraphs[16].
 
 

Enhancing Learning Through Working Memory

 
Educators can enhance student learning and achievement by implementing strategies designed to improve working memory in the classroom. Techniques such as chunking information, utilizing multiple modalities, and providing frequent retrieval practice can empower students to focus better and retain information more effectively[17]. Moreover, minimizing distractions and incorporating brain breaks are additional strategies that can optimize working memory function, fostering a more conducive learning environment[17]. It is essential to tailor these approaches to individual student needs and specific teaching contexts, promoting a collaborative effort among educators to share successful experiences and strategies[17].
 
 

Working Memory Challenges

 
 
Despite the importance of working memory in academic success, many students face challenges in this area. Populations such as children with developmental disorders, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and dyslexia, often exhibit distinctive working memory deficits. Research has shown that these deficits can manifest in various subsystems of working memory, impacting their learning processes[3]. For instance, children with dyslexia may struggle with the phonological loop, while those with ADHD may have difficulties with the central executive component[3]. Understanding these challenges is crucial for developing effective educational strategies tailored to support students with working memory impairments.
 
 

Strategies for Improvement

 
 
Several practical strategies can enhance working memory in educational settings. Techniques such as verbal repetition, the Protégé Effect (where students teach what they’ve learned), and spaced repetition have proven effective in boosting memory retention[18][19]. Engaging students in activities that require them to paraphrase or summarize information can also activate their working memory, thereby reinforcing their understanding of the material[18]. Additionally, adapting classroom environments and differentiating curriculum content can help accommodate the varying working memory capacities among students, providing individualized support where necessary[20].
 
 

Working Memory in Neuroscience

 
Working memory is a crucial cognitive function that allows individuals to temporarily hold and manipulate information necessary for various tasks, such as problem-solving and decision-making. Recent research has elucidated the complex neural mechanisms underlying working memory, revealing that it is not confined to a single brain region but involves the interplay of multiple structures.
 

Neural Underpinnings

 

Prefrontal Cortex

 
Pioneering studies in the 1970s and 1980s identified the prefrontal cortex as a central area for working memory, where neurons maintain information through sustained collective firing over extended periods, from seconds to minutes[21][22]. This prolonged activity enables the retention of multiple items, even in the presence of distractions.
 
 

Multiple Brain Regions

 
Emerging evidence suggests that working memory relies on the synchronous activity of various brain regions rather than being localized to one specific area. For instance, a study on forgetful mice indicated that at least two distinct areas of the brain collaborate to support working memory processes[22]. This finding challenges earlier assumptions that a single region is solely responsible for working memory and points to a more distributed neural architecture.
 
 

Role of the Hippocampus

 
 
While the prefrontal cortex plays a significant role in working memory, the hippocampus is also integral, particularly for binding information to spatial and temporal contexts. It facilitates the consolidation of memories and the creation of mental maps, thus enhancing spatial memory and supporting the retrieval of contextual cues that can trigger memory recall[23][13].
 
 

Additional Brain Structures

 
 
In addition to the prefrontal cortex and hippocampus, other brain structures contribute to working memory, including the striatum, which is associated with procedural memory, and the amygdala, which plays a role in emotional memories. The cerebellum also contributes to the coordination of motor tasks and balance, which can impact working memory performance[24][23].
 
 

Interplay with Academic Performance

 
 
Research has shown that working memory is closely linked with academic success, particularly in children. Studies indicate that the quality of teacher-student relationships can significantly influence working memory and, consequently, academic performance. This underscores the dynamic and bidirectional relationship between cognitive functions and educational outcomes[1][21].
 
 

Interaction with Other Cognitive Functions

 
Working memory (WM) plays a pivotal role in a variety of cognitive processes, including language comprehension, problem-solving, reasoning, and abstract thought, making it a central focus of cognitive research[15]. It functions as a mental workspace where individuals can temporarily hold and manipulate information while engaged in other tasks[25].
 
 

Role of Attention in Working Memory

 
Attention significantly influences working memory performance. It has been suggested that attention acts as a common factor connecting working memory and general intelligence, where efficient attentional control is essential for maintaining relevant information while suppressing distractions[14][26]. The cognitive load experienced during working memory tasks is closely tied to attentional resources, as the ability to focus can dictate how well individuals can encode and retrieve information[14]. For instance, when individuals switch their attention away from a memory task to engage with concurrent activities, their performance in working memory tasks may decline, highlighting the intricate relationship between these cognitive functions[26].
 
 

Executive Functions and Working Memory

 
 
Executive functions, particularly the central executive component of working memory, are crucial for coordinating cognitive tasks. This includes shifting between tasks, inhibiting dominant responses, and selectively attending to relevant information[27- ][7]. Research indicates that deficits in executive functions, such as those seen in individuals with Alzheimer's disease, can impair the ability to perform multiple tasks simultaneously, suggesting that a well-functioning central executive is essential for effective cognitive processing[27][7]. Moreover, some researchers argue that the capacity of working memory reflects the efficiency of these executive functions, particularly the ability to manage and maintain task-relevant information amidst distractions[1].
 
 

Influence on Learning and Development

 
 
Working memory is also fundamental in the learning process. Children’s development in this area is influenced by their early relationships with adults, where positive interactions can enhance their cognitive and academic growth[1]. The ability to rehearse and manipulate information in working memory is critical for learning new concepts, and students with strong working memory skills often perform better academically[28]. Conversely, those with difficulties in working memory may struggle with information retention, particularly in educational settings, leading to increased challenges in learning[25].
 
 
 

References

 
 
[1]: Frontiers | The association between working memory, teacher-student ...
Frontiers | The association between working memory, teacher-student relationship, and academic performance in primary school children (frontiersin.org)

Frontiers | The association between working memory, teacher-student relationship, and academic performance in primary school chi

IntroductionEarly relationships with teachers play an important role in children’s development and significantly influence students’ cognitive and academic p...

www.frontiersin.org

 
 
 
[2]: Working Memory Model (Baddeley and Hitch) - Simply Psychology
Working Memory Model In Psychology (Baddeley & Hitch) (simplypsychology.org)

Working Memory Model In Psychology (Baddeley & Hitch)

The Working Memory Model, proposed by Baddeley and Hitch in 1974, describes short-term memory as a system with multiple components. It comprises the central executive, which controls attention and coordinates the phonological loop (handling auditory inform

www.simplypsychology.org

 
 
 
[3]: Frontiers | Working Memory From the Psychological and Neurosciences ...
Frontiers | Working Memory From the Psychological and Neurosciences Perspectives: A Review (frontiersin.org)

Frontiers | Working Memory From the Psychological and Neurosciences Perspectives: A Review

Since the concept of working memory was introduced over 50 years ago, different schools of thought have offered different definitions for working memory base...

www.frontiersin.org

 
 
 
[4]: The Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Working Memory: A Mini Review
Frontiers | The Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Working Memory: A Mini Review (frontiersin.org)

Frontiers | The Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Working Memory: A Mini Review

A prominent account of prefrontal cortex (PFC) function is that single neurons within the PFC maintain representations of task-relevant stimuli in working me...

www.frontiersin.org

 
 
[5]: How Is Working Memory Training Likely to Influence Academic Performance ...
Frontiers | How Is Working Memory Training Likely to Influence Academic Performance? Current Evidence and Methodological Considerations (frontiersin.org)

Frontiers | How Is Working Memory Training Likely to Influence Academic Performance? Current Evidence and Methodological Conside

Working memory (WM) is one of our core cognitive functions, allowing us to keep information in mind for shorter periods of time and then work with this infor...

www.frontiersin.org

 
 
 
[6]: Understanding the Working Memory Model: Key Concepts and Applications ...
Understanding the Working Memory Model: Key Concepts and Applications – Dyslexia Campus News Magazine

Understanding the Working Memory Model: Key Concepts and Applications

The working memory model is crucial for learning, problem-solving, and decision-making. It consists of the central executive, phonological loop, and visuospatial sketchpad, impacting cognitive perf…

dyslexiacampus.com

 
 
 
[7]: Baddeley’s Model of Working Memory - ScienceBeta
Baddeley's Model of Working Memory (sciencebeta.com)

Baddeley's Model of Working Memory

Alan Baddeley and Graham Hitch presented the Baddeley model of working memory in 1974 in an attempt to give a more accurate model of primary memory (also

sciencebeta.com

 
 
 
[8]: Baddeley & Hitch’s Working Memory Model - Psychology Unlocked
Baddeley & Hitch’s Working Memory Model – Psychology Unlocked

Baddeley & Hitch’s Working Memory Model – Psychology Unlocked

QUICK READ In 1974, Baddeley & Hitch presented the Working Memory model – a new theory of primary memory, which distinguished itself from previous theories by splitting up the memory store into multiple components. The Working Memory Model divides primar

www.psychologyunlocked.com

 
 
 
[9]: The Working Memory Model (WMM) - Revision World
The Working Memory Model (WMM) | Revision World

The Working Memory Model (WMM) | Revision World

The section explores The Working Memory Model. The Working Memory Model (WMM), proposed by Baddeley and Hitch (1974), is an explanation of short-term memory that expands upon the simpler structure outlined in the Multi-Store Model. Unlike the concept of a

revisionworld.com

 
 
 
[10]: Working Memory - Psychology Today
Working Memory | Psychology Today

Working Memory

Working memory is a form of memory that allows a person to temporarily hold a limited amount of information at the ready for immediate mental use. It is considered essential for learning, problem-solving, and other mental processes.

www.psychologytoday.com

 
 
 
[11]: Baddeley's Model of Working Memory - Psynso
Baddeley's Model of Working Memory - Psynso

Baddeley's Model of Working Memory - Psynso

Schematic of Baddeley’s Model   Alan Baddeley and Graham Hitch proposed a model of working memory in 1974, in an attempt to describe a more accurate model of short-term memory. Baddeley & Hitch proposed their tripartite working memory model as an altern

psynso.com

 
 
 
[12]: CHAPTER 5: SHORT-TERM AND WORKING MEMORY - Baylor University
Chapter 5. Short-Term and Working Memory – Cognition (baylor.edu)

Chapter 5. Short-Term and Working Memory – Cognition

CHAPTER 5: SHORT-TERM AND WORKING MEMORY Working memory is like your mind’s workspace— a limited capacity system for storage and processing of information. While early researchers focused on the storage function of working memory, contemporary scientis

openbooks.library.baylor.edu

 
 
 
[13]: Working memory - Wikipedia
Working memory - Wikipedia

Working memory - Wikipedia

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Cognitive system for temporarily holding information Working memory is a cognitive system with a limited capacity that can hold information temporarily.[1] It is important for reasoning and the guidance of decision-mak

en.wikipedia.org

 
 
 
[14]: About the distinction between working memory and short-term memory
Frontiers | About the Distinction between Working Memory and Short-Term Memory (frontiersin.org)

Frontiers | About the Distinction between Working Memory and Short-Term Memory

The theoretical concepts short-term memory (STM) and working memory (WM) have been used to refer to the maintenance and the maintenance plus manipulation of ...

www.frontiersin.org

 
 
 
[15]: Role of Prefrontal Persistent Activity in Working Memory
Frontiers | Role of Prefrontal Persistent Activity in Working Memory (frontiersin.org)

Frontiers | Role of Prefrontal Persistent Activity in Working Memory

The prefrontal cortex is activated during working memory, as evidenced by fMRI results in human studies and neurophysiological recordings in animal models. P...

www.frontiersin.org

 
 
 
[16]: Boost Working Memory in Students Now: Strategies That Work!
Boost Working Memory in Students Now: Strategies That Work! - Teach Find

Boost Working Memory in Students Now: Strategies That Work! - Teach Find

Use visual aids like charts and diagrams to help students better organize and retain information. Implement brief, regular memory exercises such as chunking activities that help break down information into manageable parts. Encourage active engagement thro

teachfind.com

 
 
 
[17]: 7 Powerful Working Memory Hacks to Skyrocket Student Focus
Boost Working Memory in Students Now: Strategies That Work! - Teach Find

Boost Working Memory in Students Now: Strategies That Work! - Teach Find

Use visual aids like charts and diagrams to help students better organize and retain information. Implement brief, regular memory exercises such as chunking activities that help break down information into manageable parts. Encourage active engagement thro

teachfind.com

 
 
 
[18]: Put Working Memory to Work in Learning - Edutopia
Put Working Memory to Work in Learning | Edutopia

Put Working Memory to Work in Learning

Strengthen your students' conscious processing of information with techniques like repetition, gamification, visualization, emphasizing relevance, and peer teaching.

www.edutopia.org

 
 
 
[19]: 15 ways to maximize students' memory - InnerDrive
15 ways to maximise students' memory | InnerDrive

15 ways to maximise students' memory | InnerDrive

Enhance your students' memory and ultimately improve their exam performance with these 15 practical strategies to help them engage, learn and remember more

www.innerdrive.co.uk

 
 
 
[20]: Working Memory: Ideas for the Classroom - Ed Psych Insight
Working Memory: Ideas for the Classroom (epinsight.com)

Working Memory: Ideas for the Classroom

Susan Gathercole and Tracy Alloway describe Working Memory as the ability to hold and manipulate information in mind for a short time. It is the mental workspace in our brain and I’ve always loved the analogy to a post-it-note. Short-Term Memory and Work

www.epinsight.com

 
 
 
[21]: A revised map of where working memory resides in the brain
The Rockefeller University » A revised map of where working memory resides in the brain

A revised map of where working memory resides in the brain - News

Working memory: it’s how you make a mental shopping list without forgetting the milk, or memorize a number just long enough to write it down. But working memory is more than a prerequisite for a successful errand—the ability to briefly hold information

www.rockefeller.edu

 
 
 
[22]: A Revised Map of Where Working Memory Resides in the Brain
A Revised Map of Where Working Memory Resides in the Brain - Neuroscience News

A Revised Map of Where Working Memory Resides in the Brain - Neuroscience News

Working memory isn't confined to one area of the brain. It requires synchronous activity of at least two brain areas.

neurosciencenews.com

 
 
 
[23]: 8.3: Brain Structures in Memory
8.3: Brain Structures in Memory – Biological Psychology [Revised Edition] (pressbooks.pub)

8.3: Brain Structures in Memory – Biological Psychology [Revised Edition]

8.3: Brain Structures in Memory Given the various types of memory and underlying processes, it’s perhaps unsurprising that memory engages many brain regions. In this section, we present an overview of some brain structures involved in memory and their fu

rotel.pressbooks.pub

 
 
 
[24]: Function of Brain Regions: Key Areas and Their Roles in Cognitive ...
Function of Brain Regions: Key Areas and Their Roles in Cognitive Processes – Psych News Daily

Function of Brain Regions: Key Areas and Their Roles in Cognitive Processes

Understanding Brain Structure and Its Functions The human brain is an intricate organ that governs cognition, behavior, and bodily functions.Its sophisticated structure comprises distinct regions and cellular elements that collaborate to process informatio

psychnewsdaily.com

 
 
 
[25]: Short-Term Memory vs. Working Memory - What's the Difference? | This vs ...
Short-Term Memory vs. Working Memory - What's the Difference? | This vs. That (thisvsthat.io)

Short-Term Memory vs. Working Memory - What's the Difference? | This vs. That

What's the difference between Short-Term Memory and Working Memory? Short-term memory and working memory are both cognitive processes involved in the storage...

thisvsthat.io

 
 
 
[26]: Fluctuations of Attention and Working Memory
Fluctuations of Attention and Working Memory | Journal of Cognition

Fluctuations of Attention and Working Memory | Journal of Cognition

The Journal of Cognition, the official journal of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology, publishes reviews, empirical articles (including registered reports), data reports, stimulus development reports, comments, and methodological notes relevant t

journalofcognition.org

 
 
 
[27]: Baddeley's model of working memory - Wikipedia
Baddeley's model of working memory - Wikipedia

Baddeley's model of working memory - Wikipedia

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Model of human memory Baddeley's model of working memory is a model of human memory proposed by Alan Baddeley and Graham Hitch in 1974, in an attempt to present a more accurate model of primary memory (often referred t

en.wikipedia.org

 
 
 
[28]: Working Memory: A teacher's guide - Structural Learning
Classroom Practices of Teachers | Working Memory: A Teacher's Guide | Conclusion (structural-learning.com)

Working Memory: A teacher's guide

What is working memory, and why should we consider it when planning and delivering classroom lessons?

www.structural-learning.com

 
 
 
 
 
 
Generated in
https://storm.genie.stanford.edu/

https://storm.genie.stanford.edu/

storm.genie.stanford.edu

 
 
Stanford University Open Virtual Assistant Lab
 
 
The generated report can make mistakes. Please consider checking important information. The generated content does not represent the developer's viewpoint.

반응형

'AI' 카테고리의 다른 글

Cultures, Sequent Occupance  (0) 2025.01.08
The interplay of Cultures  (0) 2025.01.08
Constellation Story  (0) 2025.01.08
Origin of Ethics  (0) 2025.01.05
Group Foraging  (0) 2025.01.05

댓글